top of page
Writer's pictureOdonaTech

Cognitive biases that affect investors' decisions

Investment decisions are often influenced by various cognitive biases that can lead to suboptimal choices. These biases, which are systematic deviations from rational judgment, affect how investors assess information, make decisions, and manage their portfolios. Behavioral finance studies these biases to better understand investor behavior and financial market anomalies.


Les biais cognitifs dans la finance comportementale selon Midjourney
Les biais cognitifs dans la finance comportementale selon Midjourney

Here is an overview of some key cognitive biases that influence investment decisions and how they can impact investors' financial outcomes.


  1. Confirmation Bias


Confirmation bias is a cognitive phenomenon where individuals seek, interpret, and remember information in a way that confirms their preexisting beliefs, while ignoring contradictory information. The concept, theorized by Peter Wason in 1960 in his work "On the Failure to Eliminate Hypotheses in a Conceptual Task," is pervasive in human thinking and often leads to judgment errors.


In behavioral finance, this bias drives investors to favor information that supports their initial opinions about financial assets, while neglecting information that contradicts those opinions. Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky explored this bias in their research, particularly in "Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases" (1982). In finance, confirmation bias can lead to irrational decisions, such as the formation of speculative bubbles or reluctance to sell declining stocks. Understanding this bias is crucial for making more objective and rational financial decisions.


Examples of Confirmation Bias

(Enable subtitles to view the video in English)





2. Anchoring Bias

 

Anchoring bias is a cognitive phenomenon where individuals place excessive weight on the first piece of information (or "anchor") they receive when making decisions, which disproportionately influences their subsequent judgments. This bias was theorized by psychologists Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman in the 1970s. Their seminal study "Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases," published in 1974, explores this concept and other cognitive biases that affect human decision-making.


In behavioral finance, anchoring bias manifests when investors cling to specific reference values, such as the initial purchase price of a stock, and use these values to evaluate future decisions. For instance, an investor may be reluctant to sell a stock at a price lower than its initial purchase price, even if market conditions suggest that doing so would be a rational decision. This bias can lead to suboptimal investment decisions and financial losses.


Understanding anchoring bias is crucial for investors aiming to improve their decision-making strategies. It helps them recognize and mitigate the undue influence of initial information, fostering a more objective and rational approach to evaluating investments and associated risks.


Further Examples of Anchoring Bias (Enable subtitles to view the video in English)





3. Representativeness Bias


Representativeness Bias, a common cognitive trap in behavioral finance, leads us to judge the quality of an item or trend based on limited and often unrepresentative examples of reality. Investors may be swayed by a company's series of past successes without considering other crucial factors. For example, a bank that has won the "Best Bank" award three consecutive years might be perceived as a reliable choice, even though this award might reflect only a very partial aspect of its performance. This bias, reinforced by confirmation bias which encourages us to favor information that supports our beliefs, results in an oversimplified view of reality and hasty decisions. By relying on stereotypes or simplified mental models, we risk ignoring relevant information and underestimating risks. To avoid falling into this trap, it is essential to diversify information sources, question initial impressions, and adopt a more rigorous approach to analyzing investment opportunities. This approach allows for more informed decisions and reduces the risk of financial losses.


Further Examples of Representativeness Bias (Enable subtitles to view the video in English)





4. Loss Aversion


Loss aversion, a fundamental concept in Prospect Theory developed by Kahneman and Tversky, illustrates the tendency of individuals to prioritize avoiding losses over maximizing gains. This powerful cognitive bias significantly influences investor behavior in financial markets.


In practice, this phenomenon manifests in several ways:


  1. Retention of Unprofitable Assets: Investors may cling to losing investments, hoping for a turnaround even in the face of bleak prospects.

  2. Excessive Caution Towards Gains: Loss aversion can lead to a systematic underestimation of profit opportunities, favoring an excessively conservative approach.

  3. Premature Sale of Profitable Assets: Paradoxically, when an investment yields profits, the fear of losing those gains can lead to an early sale, limiting potential growth.


This asymmetry in the perception of losses and gains, where the pain of a loss outweighs the pleasure of an equivalent gain, can result in suboptimal investment decisions. For example, an investor might reject an opportunity with a 60% chance of gaining €1,000 and a 40% chance of losing €500, even though the expected value is positive.


To mitigate the impact of this bias, investors can adopt strategies such as setting clear long-term goals, using automatic stop-loss orders, and diversifying their portfolios. A thorough understanding of loss aversion not only improves individual decision-making but also helps explain certain market anomalies.


5. Status Quo Bias


Status quo bias is a cognitive phenomenon where individuals show a preference for maintaining their current situation rather than making decisions that could lead to change. The concept was theorized by psychologists William Samuelson and Richard Zeckhauser in their 1988 study "Status Quo Bias in Decision Making." This bias is characterized by a resistance to change, even when alternatives might offer significant benefits.


In behavioral finance, status quo bias leads investors to keep their investment portfolios unchanged, avoiding the reevaluation or modification of their assets despite market fluctuations or changes in their personal financial situation. This inertia can result in suboptimal performance and the persistence of unprofitable positions. Understanding status quo bias is crucial for investors as it helps them overcome the natural tendency toward inaction and adopt a more proactive and flexible approach to managing their investments, potentially improving their long-term financial outcomes.



Conclusion


At Odonatech, we understand the impact of cognitive biases on investors' decision-making and their potential to lead to suboptimal financial outcomes.


That's why we have developed innovative solutions to help investors overcome these biases. Our financial advisory tools are designed to provide objective analyses and recommendations based on behavioral data, minimizing the influence of emotional and cognitive biases on investments.


With advanced algorithms and artificial intelligence, we offer a clear and rational perspective, enabling investors worldwide, regardless of their income levels, to make more informed and balanced decisions.


With Odonatech, investors can navigate the complex investment landscape with confidence and peace of mind, optimizing their long-term financial performance.


References


Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A., (1974). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science, 185(4157), pp.1124-1131.


Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A., (1982). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.


Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A., (1979). Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica, 47(2), pp.263-291.


Samuelson, W. and Zeckhauser, R., (1988). Status Quo Bias in Decision Making. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 1(1), pp.7-59.


Wason, P.C., (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(3), pp.129-140.








Comments


bottom of page